|
|
Year : 2014 | Volume
: 7
| Issue : 2 | Page : 96-103 |
|
Effect of trataka on cognitive functions in the elderly |
|
Shubhada Talwadkar, Aarti Jagannathan, Nagarathna Raghuram
Division of Yoga and Life Sciences, Swami Vivekananda Yoga Anusandhana Samasthana, Bengaluru, Karnataka, India
Click here for correspondence address and email
Date of Web Publication | 5-Jun-2014 |
|
|
 |
|
Abstract | | |
Background: Trataka, a type of yoga practice is considered to improve cognitive functions. The aim of this study was to test the effect of trataka on cognitive functions of the elderly. Materials and Methods: Elderly subjects were recruited based on inclusion and exclusion criteria (n = 60) and randomly divided using randomized block design into two groups: Trataka and wait list control group. Trataka (a visual cleansing technique) was given for a period of 1 month (26 days). The subjects in both groups were assessed on day 1 (pre- and postintervention in trataka group and after quiet sitting in control group) and on day 30 on Digit Span Test, Six Letter Cancellation Test (SLCT), and Trail Making Test-B (TMT-B). Results: Friedman's test and Wilcoxon signed-rank test showed that at the 2 nd follow-up there was significant improvement in digit span scores (z = −3.35, P < 0.01) in the trataka group. SLCT scores (t = 5.08, P < 0.01) and TMT-B scores (t = −4.26, P < 0.01) improved immediately after the practice of trataka (when baseline compared to first follow-up). At 1 month follow-up, trataka group showed significantly better performance in the SLCT test compared to baseline (t = −3.93, P < 0.01) and TMT-B scores (t = 7.09, P < 0.01). Repeated measure analysis of variance (RM ANOVA) results also reiterated that there was significant interaction effect at the end of 1 month of trataka intervention as compared to control group on TMT-B and SLCT scores. Conclusions: The results of this study establish that Trataka can be used as a technique to enhance cognition in the elderly. Keywords: Cognitive functions; elderly; trataka.
How to cite this article: Talwadkar S, Jagannathan A, Raghuram N. Effect of trataka on cognitive functions in the elderly. Int J Yoga 2014;7:96-103 |
Introduction | |  |
In normal aging, decreased ability to retrieve information can cause memory lapses that sometimes impair the ability to perform activities of daily living. [1] These changes are largely the result of decline in frontal lobe function, which is measured as executive functions (the ability to organize, plan, and focus on a topic). [2] Age-related decline in cognitive abilities varies considerably across individuals and across cognitive domains. Various cognitive domains show different degrees of susceptibilities to aging. [3] Changes in the brain due to aging occur earliest in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). [4] The PFC is associated with memory, attention, executive functions, as well as various other complex cognitive functions. [5],[6],[7] Varied treatment options have been propagated for cognitive impairment in the elderly such as oral medications, [8] cognitive interventions, [9] diet, [10] etc., Many experts have suggested that mentally challenging activities (e.g. crossword puzzles and brain teasers) may be helpful for patients with mild cognitive impairment. [11] Physical activity (PA), aimed at improving cardiorespiratory health, has been proposed to be a good, practical, and powerful candidate to overcome cerebral and behavioral declines. [12] Yoga practices also have shown various health benefits including the ability to improve cognition and thereby preventing cognitive impairments and dementia. [13]
Many scientific studies have proven that yoga is effective to improve various cognitive functions such as remote memory, mental balance, attention and concentration, attention span, processing speed, attention alternation ability, delayed and immediate recall, executive functions, verbal retention, and recognition tests in the healthy young subjects. [14],[15] Very few studies have looked at the effect of yoga in the elderly population. For example, relaxation response training is seen to improve reaction time on simple attention/psychomotor tasks in older adults. [16] Another cross-sectional study comparing the cognitive performance of 20 meditators (long-term practitioners of Vihangam yoga meditation) and 20 non-meditators in the geriatric age group showed that Vihangam yogis performed better on all the tests of attention except for the digit backward test. [17]
The above review depicts that the number of published literature in the last decade on effectiveness of yoga on cognitive functions of the elderly is limited. Although there are no published scientific studies, authentic traditional texts of yoga describe the benefits of trataka on a whole range of physiological and cognitive functions. [18],[19] It is observed to be most effective on the ajna chakra [20] (the vortice of vital energy in the forehead) and the brain. [21] Ajna chakra is described as the "eye of knowledge", and it is said that with the activation of this chakra, the intelligence, concentration, and memory improve and the mind becomes strong and steady. [22] It is proposed that the practice of Trataka may activate this chakra, [23] and thus may improve attention, memory, and concentration. [24],[25] In the above context, the aim of this study was to test the effect of trataka on cognitive functions of the elderly. Effect of trataka was studied as against wait list control group; in improving short term memory, attention, concentration, and executive functions.
Materials and methods | |  |
Participants were recruited from old age homes in Goa and from individuals staying in and around Ponda and Margao areas in Goa. The approval from Institutional Ethics Committee of Swami Vivekananda Yoga Anusandhana Samasthana (SVYASA) was obtained.
A total of 136 subjects were screened usinginclusion and exclusion criteria. Those who were 60-80 years of age and had minimum of fifth grade education were included in the study. Further those subjects who had (a) Any neurological disorder, (b) any psychiatric disorder, and (c) had received yoga training in last 3 months were excluded from the study. The above exclusion criteria and abnormality in hearing and vision were examined by a trained and qualified clinician. Based on this screening procedure, 75 subjects were found suitable for the study. As 15 of them declined to give consent to participate, the remaining 60 subjects were considered for the study and written informed consent was taken from them.
The sample size calculation estimated that 27 participants in each group was required to detect a clinically significant difference equivalent to an effect size of 0.75 (Cohen's d) in total memory score between the groups. A sample of 27 had 80% power to detect this difference with an alpha of 0.05 for a between-groups analysis. To account for a dropout of about 10%, a sample of 30 patients in each group was decided. Hence, it was decided to recruit a total of 60 healthy elderly sample for the study using purposive sampling. The CONSORT diagram of the flow of participants and the sociodemographic data of the participants has been provided in [Figure 1] and [Table 1], respectively. | Figure 1: The CONSORT (consolidated standards of reporting trials, Altman et al., 2001) diagram of flow of participants through each stage of the randomized trial
Click here to view |
A randomized block design was used in the study where subjects were divided into four blocks (two old ages homes comprising of one block each and two blocks of individual elderly participants from Ponda area of Goa), a sample of approximately 15 subjects comprised of one block. The lottery method of manual randomization was conducted due to the small number of blocks (n = 4). Two blocks were randomized into trataka group (intervention group) and two blocks were randomized into wait list control group.
For the intervention group, assessments were conducted on day 1 before intervention, immediately after trataka intervention on day 1, and after 1 month of trataka intervention. In the wait list control group, data was taken on day 1 before the quite sitting and after 30 min of quite sitting and at the end of 1 month. The variables used in this study were: (a) Working memory (Digit Span Forward and Backward Test [26] ), (b) attention and concentration (Six Letter Cancellation Test, SLCT [27] ), and (c) executive functions (Trail Making Test B, TMT-B [28] ). For SLCT and TMT-B, the standard procedure for translation was used; translated and back translated from Roman to Devnagiri to Roman. The tests were translated into Devnagiri for the ease of application of the tests to the local population in Goa. The Digit Span (DS) is a subtest in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-third edition (WAIS-III) [29] and has been standardized for use in an Indian population. The SLCT which measures cognitive functions such as selective and focused attention, concentration, visual scanning as well as activation and inhibition of rapid responses has been employed to assess cognitive impairments in alcoholic cirrhotic patients, [30] and to evaluate target detection deficits in patients who have undergone frontal lobectomy surgery. [31] This test has also been evaluated for its reliability and validity based on standard criteria and has standard norms for the Indian population. TMT-B is one of the most popular neuropsychological tests and is included in most test batteries, is a measure of visual scanning, complex attention, psychomotor speed mental flexibility, and executive functions. The TMT is sensitive to a variety of neurological impairments. [32],[33] Adequate test-retest reliability has been found for both Part A and Part B of the TMT in the healthy control group (r = 0.46 and 0.44, respectively), as calculated using Pearson's correlation coefficients. [34]
The intervention of trataka included set of procedures including eye exercises and gazing at the candle flame with focused attention followed by defocussing. Breathing and chanting were also included in the practice that promotes internal awareness and focusing on the activity followed by defocusing. Each session was of 30 min duration [Table 2]. Classes were conducted on everyday basis, except for Sundays and attendance was also recorded. Data of only those participants who had completed a minimum of 75% and above class attendance was analyzed.
The data at baseline was assessed for normality using Shapiro-Wilk test. As the data was found to be normal for TMT-B (trataka, statistics = 0.931, P = 0.05; control, statistics = 0.929, P = 0.09) and SLCT (trataka = 0.957, P = 0.24; control statistics = 0.946, P = 0.22) parametric tests such as paired sample t-tests, independent sample t-test, and Repeated measure analysis of variance (RM ANOVA) were used to analyze the data. In case of the Digit Span Test, the data was not found to be normally distributed (trataka statistics = 0.845, P = 0.00; control statistics = 0.931, P = 0.72). There were no outliers in the data; however the data had distinct two peaks at the higher and lower range of scores. If we had divided the digit span scores using the median values into two groups, we could have possibly got two independent normal distributions. However, as we did not have any rationale for dividing the group based on the median values, nonparametric tests such as Mann-Whitney, Wilcoxon signed rank test, and Friedman's test were used to analyze the Digit Span Test scores. Bonferroni adjustment was conducted as there were multiple comparisons to analyze the time effect.
Results | |  |
Out of the 60 subjects, 55 subjects completed the first follow-up on day 1 (postintervention). However, only 48 subjects completed the second follow-up, which was conducted at the end of 1 month (trataka group n = 26, wait list control n = 22).
There were no group differences at baseline in all three outcome variables. Both the groups were comparable at baseline, on all outcome variables. On Mann-Whitney tests, there was no significant difference between trataka and wait list control group in Digit Span Test scores at the first follow-up. When compared to wait list control group at the second follow-up a possible trend towards significance could be observed in the trataka group. When compared within group (Friedman's test and Wilcoxon signed rank test), digit span scores improved at the first follow-up in trataka group, but the difference was not significant. At the second follow-up, there was significant improvement in digit span scores (z = −3.35, P < 0.01). While in control group, scores decreased at the first and the second follow-up, but there were no any significant changes [Figure 2]. | Figure 2: Changes in median digit span scores across timeline in trataka and wait list control group (Friedman's test)
Click here to view |
When compared between groups (independent sample t-test), there was no significant difference between the trataka and wait list control group at first and second follow-up in SLCT scores. However, with respect to time effect (paired sample t-test), selective as well as sustained attention and concentration (measured using SLCT scores) was seen to improve immediately after the practice of trataka (when baseline compared to first follow-up) (t = 5.08, P < 0.01). Wait list control group also performed better (may be because of retest effect), but the improvement was not significant. At 1 month follow-up, trataka group showed significantly better performance in the SLCT test compared to baseline (t = −3.93, P < 0.01). Whereas, scores of wait list control group came back to the baseline scores at the second follow-up.
On the independent sample t-test, at the first follow-up, there was no significant difference in TMT-B scores between trataka and wait list control group. However at the second follow-up, there was a trend towards significance. Trataka group performed significantly better at the first follow-up (paired sample t-test) (t = −4.26, P < 0.01) in TMT B test (indicative of executive functions). In contrast, in the wait list control group there was increase in time taken to complete the task (suggestive of poor performance) and the change was not significant. At the second follow-up, only trataka group showed significantly improved (statistics = 7.09, P = 0.00) performance when compared to the baseline scores.
The traditional analysis that is used to detect treatment outcomes in randomized longitudinal clinical trials was used; RM ANOVA. RM ANOVA results showed that the executive functions in both the groups improved over time (occasion effect). Though there was no significant group effect, trataka group showed significant improvement in TMT-B scores over a month period of the study as compared to the wait list control group (f = 6.67, P < 0.01; interaction effect) [Table 3]. With respect to SLCT scores, there was a trend towards better improvement (f = 3.11, P = 0.05) in trataka group as compared to the wait list control group over the 1 month period of the study (interaction effect) [Table 3].
For the convenience of conducting intervention, half of the subjects in the trataka group were provided the intervention in the morning and the remaining half were provided the intervention in the evening. Baseline analysis showed that there were significant differences in TMT-B and SLCT scores between the morning group and the evening group, with scores higher in the evening group than morning group. Hence, post hoc test was conducted to check if there was any significant interaction effect (group X time) after controlling for baseline scores using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Results show that there was no significant difference between groups (people who practiced in the morning as compared to people who practiced in the evening) over the study period after controlling for baseline differences.
Discussion | |  |
In this study on elderly subjects, trataka intervention improved cognitive functions (short-term memory and working memory, selective and focused attention, concentration, visual scanning as well as activation and inhibition of rapid responses and executive functions) when compared to wait list control group at the end of 1 month. Trataka practice involves various steps like preparatory eye exercises, focusing, defocusing, chanting, and silence during relaxation. Each component or all of them together could have been responsible for the improvement in the cognitive functions. Preparatory eye exercises improve the stamina of the eye muscles and avoid eye strain. The degree of optical illusion is observed to reduce post a set of yoga practices that includes trataka (involving both focusing and defocusing of the gaze and attention). [35] Dharana or focusing improves concentrative attention ("desha-bandhashchittasya dharanam0"; Patanjali's Yoga Sutras, Chapter III, Verse 1). [36] Focused attention (FA) is the attention which is restricted to a specific focus [37] such as the breath or the candle flame (trataka). Receptive attention is a kind of attention which is "objectless" and the goal is simply to keep attention fully "readied" in the present moment of experience without orienting, directing, or limiting it in any way. Research studies have shown that intense FA meditation effects cortical engagement, as reflected by a concomitant reduction in event related desynchronization (ERD) to target tones in the beta (13-30 Hz) frequency band. Reductions in beta ERD after practice of external tasks is due to the decreased cognitive efforts. [38] There is enhanced processing of task-related auditory inputs during FA meditation. FA meditation training is thought to improve one's ability to remain vigilant and monitor distractors without losing focus. It is proposed that these mental training-related effects might be produced by a reduction in cortical noise and/or by an enhancement of the rhythmic mode of attention.
The second stage of trataka, the phase of defocussing is akin to the stage of dhyana effortless attention ("tatra pratyayaikatanata dhyanam"; Patanjali's Yoga Sutras, Chapter III, Verse 2). [39] When dharana becomes effortless, it takes the form of dhyana, which is defined as the uninterrupted spontaneous flow of the mind toward the chosen object. Vigilance and attention are not required during dhyana, which is the actual phase of meditation. [40] Though there are different forms of meditation all of them lead to calm yet alert mind. [41] At a more advanced level of training in FA meditation which could be considered a state of dhyana, the regulative attention skills are invoked less frequently, and the ability to sustain focus thus becomes progressively "effortless". [38] Dhyana is associated with reduced sympathetic activity and increased vagal tone. [42] The defocussed phase of trataka could be similar to the benefits of dhyana phase of meditation. Multiple studies show that meditation may affect multiple pathways that could play a role in brain aging and mental fitness. [13] For example, meditation may reduce stress-induced cortisol secretion and this could have neuroprotective effects potentially via elevating levels of brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). Meditation processes are linked to gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) ergic cortical inhibition, a mechanism implicated in improved cognitive performance and enhanced emotional regulation. [43] Further, meditation may potentially strengthen neuronal circuits and enhance cognitive reserve capacity. Brain regions associated with attention, interception, and sensory processing are thicker in meditation practitioners including the PFC and right anterior insula. [44] Advanced meditators have higher melatonin levels (that blocks the build-up of beta-amyloid plaque, a hallmark feature of Alzheimer's disease) [45] than nonmeditators. [46]
The results suggest that long-term practice of trataka and not just 1 day practice is required to improve short-term memory. Similar study done on elderly subjects showed that, at the 3 month follow-up, yoga group improved in semantic memory, short-term primary memory, and short-term working memory. [47] So, the result of our study is consistent with the results of earlier study. The only difference is that our study period was only of 1 month; still we could show significant improvement in the trataka group. Thus, we can make a statement from our results that first time yoga participants, if provided with 1 month trataka intervention, can improve their executive, memory, and cognitive functioning. This claim however needs to be tested in larger samples.
In a study done on the healthy aging adults, it was seen that performance on a simple attention task improved after 5-week relaxation response training program; whereas, no improvement was seen in complex tasks of attention. [16] In another study, net scores on the six-letter cancellation task were significantly higher after a session of Dharana.[48] These results are in consistence with our results, as we also observed increased SLCT scores immediately after Trataka practice. Since Trataka is a type of dharana practice that involves focused attention on a specified object, this further strengthens that the results are valid and obtained correctly.
There could be various other possible reasons for finding differences over the 1 month period. The group was at a stage when cognitive decline was a reality. All the aging individuals (after the age of 60 years) develop some degree of decline in cognitive capacity as time progresses. Studies show that 16.8% of aged people have some form of cognitive decline without the symptoms of dementia. [49] If the study was done on healthy young subjects, then we might not have got the significant difference, because of 'ceiling effect'.
Another reason for the significant result could be that majority of the participants of the study had never been exposed to trataka or any yoga intervention earlier. A few of the participants, who had earlier learnt yoga, had either discontinued or had not practiced it for the past 3 months. In such a case, we believe that the effect of trataka was pronounced as there was no previous or past effect of any similar intervention.
The fact that we got significant results to show that trataka practice for 1 month is effective in improving cognitive functions shows that the scales used in this study were sensitive enough to tap the cognitive improvement in the elderly after the trataka intervention. Three tests used in this study were Digit Span Test, TMT-B, and SLCT. Though not developed specifically to test the effect of trataka, these widely used tests have shown that they can tap significant changes post yoga intervention.
Studies have time and again discussed the importance of the prolonged practice of yoga. [50],[51],[52] We assessed the cognitive functions immediately after one session of trataka and after 1 month of continuous daily practice. The results pronounced that there was no significant difference between groups at the end of one session (first follow-up); however, significant group and time differences including interaction effects were observed at the end of 1 month of intervention. Hence, we believe that our study results validate earlier quoted studies which advocate prolonged duration (number of days) of practice of yoga/trataka for desirable effects.
The design of the study, that is, randomized block design (RBD) was the main strength of the study. RBD eliminates any bias in treatment assignment, specifically selection bias and confounding. It maximizes statistical power, especially in subgroup analyses. Another strength of the study was that the intervention was provided to the sample that needed the intervention, aging individuals (after the age of 60), as they often develop some degree of decline in cognitive capacity.
In spite of its strong methodology, the results of the study need to be understood in the context that the sampling was done only in two old age homes in Goa. Further the sample size was small. The total sample size was 60 based on earlier sample size calculations and post attrition, the sample size for analysis was reduced to 48. The results of the current study showed that there was a trend towards significance in the trataka group in the between group analysis (group effect) for some outcome variables. In this context, a larger sample size could have depicted significant differences between groups. Also, only three outcome variables were used in the study. Age-related cognitive decline can be seen in different cognitive domains (e.g. speed of processing, spatial ability, reasoning, etc.) and varies individually. Further studies can be conducted to test the effect of trataka on different neurological test batteries.
Conclusion | |  |
The results of this study establish that trataka can be used as a technique to enhance cognition in the elderly. The trataka intervention is easy to learn, implement, and adhere. Further trataka, after the initial few sessions, can be practiced independently by the participant to achieve desired results. For researchers, this study could provide a substantial base for conducting future trials to test the efficacy of trataka in controlled experiments.
References | |  |
1. | Squire LR. Memory and Brain. New York: Oxford Univ Pr; AQ71987.  |
2. | Schacter DL. Stories of Elders, Searching for Memory: The Brain, the Mind, and the Past. New York: Basic Books; 1997. p. 280-AQ9308.  |
3. | Hughes TF. Promotion of cognitive health through cognitive activity in the aging population. Aging Health 2010;6:111-21.  [PUBMED] |
4. | Smith EE, Jonides J. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 1999;283:1657-61.  |
5. | Gray JR, Braver TS, Raichle ME. Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002;99:4115-20.  |
6. | Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal lobe" tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000;41:49-100.  |
7. | West RL. An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychol Bull 1996;120:272-92.  [PUBMED] |
8. | Panza F, Frisardi V, Capurso C, D'Introno A, Colacicco AM, Chiloiro R, et al. Effect of donepezil on the continuum of depressive symptoms, mild cognitive impairment, and progression to dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 2010;58:389-90.  [PUBMED] |
9. | Simon SS, Yokomizo JE, Bottino CM. Cognitive intervention in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2012;36:1163-78.  |
10. | Roberts RO, Geda YE, Cerhan JR, Knopman DS, Cha RH, Christianson TJ, et al. Vegetables, unsaturated fats, moderate alcohol intake, and mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010;29:413-23.  |
11. | Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Aggarwal NT, Boyle PA, Hebert LE, Mendes de Leon CF, et al. Cognitive activity and the cognitive morbidity of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2010;75:990-6.  |
12. | Abou-Dest A, Albinet CT, Boucard G, Audiffren M. Swimming as a positive moderator of cognitive aging: A cross-sectional study with a multitask approach. J Aging Res 2012;2012:273185.  |
13. | Xiong GL, Doraiswamy PM. Does meditation enhance cognition and brain plasticity? Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1172:63-9.  |
14. | Chattha R, Nagarathna R, Padmalatha V, Nagendra HR. Effect of yoga on cognitive functions in climacteric syndrome: A randomized control study. BJOG 2008;115:991-1000.  |
15. | Prakash R, Dubey I, Abhishek P, Gupta SK, Rastogi P, Siddiqui SV. Long-term Vihangam Yoga meditation and scores on tests of attention. Percept Mot Skills 2010;110:1139-48.  |
16. | Galvin JA, Benson H, Deckro GR, Fricchione GL, Dusek JA. The relaxation response: Reducing stress and improving cognition in healthy aging adults. Complement Ther Clin Pract 2006;12:186-91.  |
17. | Prakash R, Rastogi P, Dubey I, Abhishek P, Chaudhury S, Small BJ. Long-term concentrative meditation and cognitive performance among older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2012;19:479-94.  |
18. | Niranjanananda SS, Gheranda S. Commentary on the Yoga Teachings of Maharshri Gheranda. Yoga Publications Trust: Munger, India, 2012.  |
19. | Muktibodhananda S, Saraswati SS. Hatha Yoga Pradipika. Yoga Publications Trust: Munger; 2000.  |
20. | Gharote ML, Parimal D, Jha VK. Hatharatnavali-a treatise on hathayoga. Lonavala: The Lonavla Yoga Institute; 2002.  |
21. | Niranjanananda SS. Dharana darshan-yogic, Tantric and Upanishadic Practices of Concentration and Visualizations. 2 nd ed. Munger, Bihar: Yoga Publications Trust; 2003.  |
22. | Redmond L. Chakra meditation: Tranformation Through the Seven Energy Centres of the Body. Canada: Sounds True; 2010.  |
23. | Niranjanananda SS. Yoga Darshan-vision of the Yoga Upanishads. Munger: Yoga Publications trust; 2002.  |
24. | Nagendra HR, Nagrathana R. Promotion of Positive Health. Bangalore: Swami Vivekananda Yoga Prakashana; 2001.  |
25. | Niranjanananda SS. Dharana darshan-yogic, Tantric and Upanishadic Practices of Concentration and Visualizations. 2 nd ed. Munger: Yoga Publications Trust; 2003.  |
26. | Wechsler DA. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-third edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.  |
27. | Diller L, Ben YY, Gerstman LJ, Goodin R, Gordon W, Weinberg J. Studies in scanning behavior in hemiplegia. Rehabilitation Monograph No. 50, Studies in cognition and rehabilitation in hemiplegia. New York: New York University Medical Center, Institute of Rehabilitation Medicine; 1974.  |
28. | Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead-ReitanNeuropsycholgical Test Battery: Therapy and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson: Neuropsychological Press; 1985.  |
29. | Wechsler DA. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3 rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.  |
30. | Kapczinski F, Curran HV, Przemioslo R, Williams R, Fluck E, Fernandes C, et al. Cognitive impairments of alcoholic cirrhotic patients: Correlation with endogenous benzodiazepine receptor ligands and increased affinity of platelet receptors. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1996;60:676-80.  |
31. | Richer F, Decary A, Lapierre MF, Rouleau I, Bouvier G, Saint-Hilaire JM. Target detection deficits in frontal lobectomy. Brain Cogn 1993;21:203-11.  |
32. | Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. 3 rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995.  |
33. | Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary. 2 nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.  |
34. | Matarazzo JD, Wiens AN, Matarazzo RG, Goldstein SG. Psychometric and clinical test-retest reliability of the Halstead impairment index in a sample of healthy, young, normal men. J Nerv Ment Dis 1974;188:37-49.  |
35. | Telles S, Nagarathna R, Vani PR, Nagendra HR. A combination of focusing and defocusing through yoga reduces optical illusion more than focusing alone. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol 1997;41:179-82.  |
36. | Taimini IK. The Science of Yoga. Madras: The Theosophical Publishing House; 2005. p. 275-8.  |
37. | Jha AP, Krompinger J, Baime MJ. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2007;7:109-19.  |
38. | Lutz A, Slagter HA, Rawlings NB, Francis AD, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ. Mental training enhances attentional stability: Neural and behavioral evidence. J Neurosci 2009;29:13418-27.  |
39. | Taimini IK. The science of yoga. Madras: The Theosophical Publishing House; 2005. p. 278-80.  |
40. | Telles S, Naveen KV, Balkrishna A. Meditation and attention: A comment on a recent article. Percept Mot Skills 2010;111:918-20.  |
41. | Telles S, Raghavendra BR, Naveen KV, Manjunath NK, Kumar S, Subramanya P. Changes in autonomic variables following two meditative states described in yoga texts. J Altern Complement Med 2013;19:35-42.  |
42. | Telles S and Bhat RR Neurophysiological Changes in Meditation Correlated with Descriptions from the Ancient Texts. 2011. p. 56-9.  |
43. | Guglietti CL, Daskalakis ZJ, Radhu N, Fitzgerald PB, Ritvo P. Meditation-related increases in GABAB modulated cortical inhibition. Brain Stimul 2013;6:397-402.  |
44. | Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman RH, Gray JR, Greve DN, Treadway MT, et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport 2005;16:1893-7.  |
45. | Feng Z, Zhang JT. Protective effect of melatonin on beta-amyloid-induced apoptosis in rat astroglioma c6 cells and its mechanism. Free Radic Biol Med 2004;37:1790-801.  |
46. | Solberg EE, Holen A, Ekeberg O, Osterud B, Halvorsen R, Sandvik L. The effects of long meditation on plasma melatonin and blood serotonin. Med Sci Monit 2004;10:CR96-101.  |
47. | Manjunath NK. Yoga and geriatric care: An evaluation of positive health indicators. Phd Thesis submitted to Swami Vivekananda Yoga Anusandhana Samasthana (SVYASA), Bangalore, India; 2005.  |
48. | Kumar S, Telles S. Meditative states based on yoga texts and their effects on performance of a letter-cancellation task. Percept Mot Skills 2009;109:679-89.  |
49. | Graham JE, Rockwood K, Beattie BL, Eastwood R, Gauthier S, Tuokko H, et al. Prevalence and severity of cognitive impairment with and without dementia in an elderly population. Lancet 1997;349:1793-6.  |
50. | Chaya MS, Kurpad AV, Nagendra HR, Nagaratna R. The effect of long term combined yoga practice on the basal metabolic rate of healthy adults. BMC Complement Altern Med 2006;6:28.  |
51. | Duraiswamy G, Thirthalli J, Nagendra HR, Gangadhar BN. Yoga therapy as an add-on treatment in the management of patients with schizophrenia:A randomized controlled trial. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2007;116:226-32.  |
52. | Gangadhar BN, Nagendra HR, Thirthalli J, Subbakrishna DK, Muralidhar D, Varambally S, et al. Efficacy of Yoga as an Add-on Treatment in Schizophrenia. Project report submitted under Scheme for Extra Mural Research (EMR) to Ayurveda, Yoga and Naturopathy, Unani, Siddha and Homoeopathy (AYUSH), New Delhi; 2010.  |

Correspondence Address: Aarti Jagannathan Swami Vivekananda Yoga Anusandhana Samasthana 19, Gavipuuram, Kempegowda Nagar 560 019, Bengaluru, Karnataka India
 Source of Support: Study conducted as part of the MD degree in
‘Yoga and Rehabilitation, at SVYASA University, Bengaluru,, Conflict of Interest: None  | Check |
DOI: 10.4103/0973-6131.133872
Clinical trial registration CTRI/2013/06/003727

[Figure 1], [Figure 2]
[Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3] |
|
This article has been cited by | 1 |
Need for development of indigenous socioculturally relevant mental health interventions from India |
|
| Aarti Jagannathan | | Journal of Applied Consciousness Studies. 2022; 10(2): 95 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 2 |
Effects of yoga-related mind-body therapies on cognitive function in older adults: A systematic review with meta-analysis |
|
| Kallol Kumar Bhattacharyya, Ross Andel, Brent J. Small | | Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 2021; 93: 104319 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 3 |
Health and therapeutic benefits of Shatkarma: A narrative review of scientific studies |
|
| P.S. Swathi, B.R. Raghavendra, Apar Avinash Saoji | | Journal of Ayurveda and Integrative Medicine. 2021; 12(1): 206 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 4 |
Trataka Kriya in Individuals with Digital Eye Strain: A Pre–Post Experimental Design |
|
| Dhanesh Kumar K. U., Sparsha Shetty, Hrishikesh Amin, Rashmitha A. P., Shilna Rani P. | | Journal of Health and Allied Sciences NU. 2021; | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 5 |
Effect of Jyoti-Trataka on intraocular pressure, autonomic control, and blood glucose in diabetic patients with high-tension primary open-angle glaucoma: a randomized-controlled trial |
|
| Ali Mohamed Ali Ismail, Hatem Fawzy Abd Elfatah Abo Saif, Alaa Mohamed El-Moatasem Mohamed | | Journal of Complementary and Integrative Medicine. 2021; 0(0) | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 6 |
Effect of Trataka (Yogic Visual Concentration) on the Performance in the Corsi-Block Tapping Task: A Repeated Measures Study |
|
| P. S. Swathi, Raghavendra Bhat, Apar Avinash Saoji | | Frontiers in Psychology. 2021; 12 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 7 |
Real-World fNIRS Brain Activity Measurements during Ashtanga Vinyasa Yoga |
|
| Henrikke Dybvik, Martin Steinert | | Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(6): 742 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 8 |
Cognitive Analytics for Rapid Stress Relief in Humans Using EEG Based Analysis of Tratak Sadhana (Meditation) |
|
| Swati Kamthekar, Prachi Deshpande, Brijesh Iyer | | International Journal of Information Retrieval Research. 2020; 10(4): 1 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 9 |
Impact of Yoga on cognition and mental health among elderly: A systematic review |
|
| Shivaji Chobe, Meenakshi Chobe, Kashinath Metri, Sanjib Kumar Patra, R Nagaratna | | Complementary Therapies in Medicine. 2020; 52: 102421 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | |
|
|
 |
 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Article Access Statistics | | Viewed | 8440 | | Printed | 192 | | Emailed | 1 | | PDF Downloaded | 403 | | Comments | [Add] | | Cited by others | 9 | |
|

|